SwePub
Tyck till om SwePub Sök här!
Sök i SwePub databas

  Extended search

Träfflista för sökning "hsv:(SAMHÄLLSVETENSKAP) hsv:(Psykologi) ;pers:(Nilsson Lars Göran);lar1:(liu)"

Search: hsv:(SAMHÄLLSVETENSKAP) hsv:(Psykologi) > Nilsson Lars Göran > Linköping University

  • Result 1-6 of 6
Sort/group result
   
EnumerationReferenceCoverFind
1.
  • Levén, Anna, 1976-, et al. (author)
  • Prospective memory and intellectual disability
  • 2017
  • Conference paper (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Previous studies have shown prospective memory errors in persons with intellectual disability (adults, Levén et al., 2007, 2008, 2011, 2013, adolescents, Meilan et al., 2009). As prospective memory failures are one of the most common memory errors (Crovitz and Daniel, 1984), this may explain part of the difficulties in handling demands in everyday life for persons with intellectual disability. Prospective memory refers to memory focused on realising intentions in the future. The ongoing task is performed as the intention is stored in long-term memory. Despite research in different clinical groups (brain injuries, Mioni et al., 2013; Aging, Kvavilashvili et al, 2013; schizophrenia, Raskin et al, 2013; ADHD,  Brandimonte et al., 2011, Kliegel, 2013, etc.) this aspect of memory has received little attention in relation to intellectual disability (adults, Levén et al., 2007, 2008, 2011, 2013, adolescents, Meilan et al., 2009).Meilan et al. (2009) got results supporting the multi-process view on prospective memory (McDaniel & Einstein, 1990). The multiprocess view on PM (Einstein & McDaniel, 1990) predicts that the prospective part will be noticed more automatic in specific circumstances, specifically, if the PM cue is either; (a) strongly associated with the planned action, (b) associated with the ongoing task, (c) salient, or (d) has relevant features that come into focus of attention as a result of processing associated with the ongoing task (M. A. McDaniel & Einstein, 2000; M. A. McDaniel, Guynn, Einstein, & Breneiser, 2004).Intellectual disability and prospective memory.Research on prospective memory has increased dramatically in recent years, although persons with intellectual disability have not been thoroughly investigated. Recent studies have studied prospective memory in persons with traumatic brain injury. Persons with intellectual disability may have suffered a brain injury in childhood, but there are also genetic and foremost unknown (about 50%) reasons for intellectual disability.Keeping appointments (Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test) have been found to be a difficult subtest for persons with intellectual disability (Martin et al., 2000; Meilan et al., 2009). The child version of the test has been used in adult persons with Down's syndrome (Aldrich et al., 1991; Wilson 1995). Performance was low also on the borrowed item subtest of the River Mead Behavioural Memory Test. In this task the experimenter borrows an item and the participant is to ask for it later on in the experiment. Prospective memory performance is strongly related to retrospective memory performance in persons with intellectual disability similar as in children. This has been attributed to a general weakness in memory processes such as episodic memory, and/or attention (Levén et al, 2008).Method.Population. The participants in the present study were a subsample drawn from the prospective Betula cohort study testing wave 2. There were 2840 people between 35 and 90 years of age. People with genetic syndromes (e.g. Down syndrome) were excluded in Betula, which gave a mixed or non-specific intellectual disabilities sample. In the Betula database, a group with intellectual disability (IQ < 70, n=58) was defined, using a verbal and a nonverbal IQ test, together with a control group matched on age, sex, level of education and years of education (n=116). Note that these groups were old (mean age = 73 years) compared to most other studies on ID.Tasks: At the beginning of all of the memory tests each day, the test leader asked the subject to remind him/her that they should sign a paper at the end of the day after all the tests were finished. Subjects were not told that this was a memory test. Together with this task on prospective memory, several tasks of episodic memory (recall, recognition), semantic memory (knowledge, fluency) and short-term memory was used.The task was scored as follows:4, recall no cue: Subjects remembered this without any aid3, after cue: Test leader: Was there not something else we should do?2, after reminding: Test leader: Were you not supposed to remind me of something?1, failed completely: Failed to remember despite of cues givenResults.See Table 1 for frequencies for the different prospective memory answers. The controls perform better than persons with intellectual disability on the prospective memory task, F(1,173) = 10.8, p<.001. However, almost all participants with ID managed to perform the task with reminders.Table 1. Frequencies for the prospective memory answers in percent in the two groups.Prospective memory answerIDControls1, failed completely14 %8 %2, recall after reminding53 %25 %3, recall after cue14 %37 %4, recall without cue19 %30 % In the control group, prospective memory correlated significantly with episodic memory (.19), semantic memory (.32) and short-term memory (.22), whereas in the intellectual disability group none of the correlations were significant (episodic memory .16, semantic memory .08, and short-term memory .18). The memory measures were also more intercorrelated in the control group compared to the intellectual disability group. This is contrary to previous results (Levén et al., 2008, 2011, 2013) where the ID group had higher intercorrelations.Discussion.Low prospective memory performance was found in the intellectual disability group as in previous studies. Prospective memory correlated with other memory functions only in the control group. One possibility is that prospective memory loads on executive functions, or vigilance not measured in this study.Persons with intellectual disability have not been able to perform verbal prospective memory tasks at all in previous studies. In this study, on the contrary, persons with intellectual disability performed a verbal prospective memory task. About half of the participants with intellectual disability remembered the prospective memory task if the experimenter asked what they should remind them of. That is, they managed the task with a cue that reduced the load on long-term memory. Previous studies have used a less explicit cue which did not improve performance significantly in the intellectual disability group.Selected ReferencesBrandimonte, M. A., Filippello, P., Coluccia, E., Altgassen, M., & Kliegel, M. (2011). To do or not to do? Prospective memory versus response inhibition in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Memory, 19(1), 56-66. doi:10.1080/09658211.2010.535657Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, (4), 717-726.Levén, A., Lyxell, B., Andersson, J., Danielsson, H., & Rönnberg, J. (2011). The relationship between prospective memory, working memory and self-rated memory performance in individuals with intellectual disability. Scandinavian Journal of Disability Research. 1-17.                                           Levén, A., Lyxell, B., Andersson, J., Danielsson, H. & Rönnberg, J. (2008). Prospective memory, working memory, retrospective memory and self-rated memory performance in persons with intellectual disability. Scandinavian Journal of Disability Research. , 10(3), 147-165.Levén, A., Lyxell, B., Andersson, J. & Danielsson, H. (2013). Pictures as cues or as support to verbal cues at encoding and execution of prospective memories in individuals with intellectual disability. Scandinavian Journal of Disability Research.Meilán, J. G., Pérez, E., Arana, J. M., & Carro, J. (2009). Neuropsychological and cognitive factors in event-based prospective memory performance in adolescents and young people with an intellectual disability. British Journal Of Developmental Disabilities, 55(108,Pt1), 61-75. doi:10.1179/096979509799103179Mioni, G., Rendell, P. G., Henry, J. D., Cantagallo, A., & Stablum, F. (2013). An investigation of prospective memory functions in people with traumatic brain injury using Virtual Week. Journal Of Clinical And Experimental Neuropsychology, 35(6), 617-630. doi:10.1080/13803395.2013.804036Wilson, B. A. and Ivani-Chalian, R. (1995), Performance of adults with Down's syndrome on the Children's Version of the Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test: A brief report. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34: 85–88. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01440.x
  •  
2.
  • Wikgren, Mikael, 1981-, et al. (author)
  • APOE ε4 is associated with longer telomeres, and longer telomeres among ε4 carriers predicts worse episodic memory
  • 2012
  • In: Neurobiology of Aging. - : Elsevier. - 0197-4580 .- 1558-1497. ; 33:2, s. 335-344
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Both leukocyte telomere length and the apolipoprotein ε4 allele have been associated with mortality, cardiovascular disease, cognition, and dementia. The authors investigated whether leukocyte telomere length was associated with APOE genotype or cognitive abilities in the context of APOE genotype. The setting for this cross-sectional study was 427 nondemented individuals aged 41–81 yr. The authors found that ε4 carriers overall exhibited significantly longer telomeres compared with non-carriers (difference of 268 bp, p = 0.001). This difference was greatest at the lower limit of the age span and nonsignificant at the upper limit, which translated into a significantly higher telomere attrition rate (p = 0.049) among ε4 carriers (37 bp/years) compared with non-carriers (21 bp/year). Further, longer telomeres among ε4 carriers significantly predicted worse performance on episodic memory tasks. No significant associations were found on tasks tapping semantic and visuospatial ability, or among ε3/ε3 carriers. In conclusion, APOE ε4 carriers had longer telomeres compared with non-carriers, but higher rate of attrition. Among them, longer telomeres predicted worse performance on episodic memory tasks. These observations suggest that the ε4 allele is associated with abnormal cell turnover of functional and possibly clinical significance.
  •  
3.
  • Magnussen, S., et al. (author)
  • What people believe about memory
  • 2006
  • In: Memory. - : Informa UK Limited. - 0965-8211 .- 1464-0686. ; 14:5, s. 595-613
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Two representative samples of adult Norwegians (n=2000) were asked a set of general and specific questions regarding their beliefs and opinions about human memory. The results indicate that on many questions, such as time of the earliest memories, inhibiting effects of collaboration, and memory for dramatic versus ordinary events, the views of the general public concurred with current research findings, and people in general had realistic views about their own memory performance. On other questions, such as the reliability of olfactory as compared with visual and auditory memory, the memory of small children in comparison with that of adults, the likelihood of repression of adult traumatic memories, and on more general questions such as the possibility of training memory and the capacity limitations of long-term memory, a large proportion of the participants expressed views that are less supported by scientific evidence. Implications of these findings are briefly discussed. © 2006 Psychology Press Ltd.
  •  
4.
  • Rönnberg, Jerker, et al. (author)
  • Hearing Loss Is Negatively Related to Episodic and Semantic Long-Term Memory but Not to Short-Term Memory
  • 2011
  • In: Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. - : American Speech Language Hearing Association. - 1092-4388 .- 1558-9102. ; 54:2, s. 705-726
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Purpose: To test the relationship between degree of hearing loss and different memory systems in hearing aid users. Method: Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to study the relationship between auditory and visual acuity and different cognitive and memory functions in an age-hetereogenous subsample of 160 hearing aid users without dementia, drawn from the Swedish prospective cohort aging study known as Betula (L.-G. Nilsson et al., 1997). Results: Hearing loss was selectively and negatively related to episodic and semantic long-term memory (LTM) but not short-term memory (STM) performance. This held true for both ears, even when age was accounted for. Visual acuity alone, or in combination with auditory acuity, did not contribute to any acceptable SEM solution. Conclusions: The overall relationships between hearing loss and memory systems were predicted by the ease of language understanding model (J. Rönnberg, 2003), but the exact mechanisms of episodic memory decline in hearing aid users (i.e., mismatch/disuse, attentional resources, or information degradation) remain open for further experiments. The hearing aid industry should strive to design signal processing algorithms that are cognition friendly.
  •  
5.
  • Wikgren, Mikael, 1981-, et al. (author)
  • Short Telomeres in Depression and the General Population Are Associated with a Hypocortisolemic State
  • 2012
  • In: Biological Psychiatry. - New York : Elsevier. - 0006-3223 .- 1873-2402. ; 71:4, s. 294-300
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Background The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis plays a central role in stress regulation, and leukocyte telomere length (TL) has been suggested to represent a cumulative measure of stress. Depression is intimately related with stress and frequently exhibits a dysregulated HPA axis. We aimed to study the relationships between TL and biological and psychological facets of stress in recurrent major depressive disorder and controls. Methods Leukocyte TL was measured in 91 subjects with recurrent major depressive disorder and 451 control subjects. Stress was assessed from both a biological perspective, by assessing HPA axis function with a weight-adjusted very-low-dose dexamethasone suppression test (DST), and a psychological perspective, with self-report questionnaires. Results TL was shorter among patients compared with control subjects (277 base pairs, p = .001). Overall, short TL was associated with a hypocortisolemic state (low post-DST cortisol and high percentage of cortisol reduction after the DST) among both patients and control subjects but more pronounced among patients. This state, which was overrepresented among patients, was characterized by high familial loading of affective disorders among patients (p = .001) and high C-reactive protein levels among control subjects (p = .040). TL was also inversely associated with stress measured with the Perceived Stress Questionnaire (rs = −.258, p = .003). Conclusions Short TL is associated with depression and hypocortisolism. Because hypocortisolism has been shown to develop from chronic stress exposure, our findings corroborate the concept of TL as a cumulative measure of stress and provide novel insights into the detrimental role of stress in depressive illness and the general population.
  •  
6.
  • Wikgren, Mikael, 1981-, et al. (author)
  • Shorter telomere length is linked to brain atrophy and white matter hyperintensities
  • 2014
  • In: Age and Ageing. - : Oxford University Press (OUP). - 0002-0729 .- 1468-2834. ; 43:2, s. 212-217
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Background: leukocyte telomere length (TL) is considered a marker of biological aging. Several studies have investigated the link between leukocyte TL and aging-associated functional attributes of the brain, but no prior study has investigated whether TL can be linked to brain atrophy and white matter hyperintensities (WMHs); two prominent structural manifestations of brain aging. Methods: we investigated whether leukocyte TL was related to brain atrophy and WMHs in a sample of 102 non-demented individuals aged 64-75 years. Results: shorter TL was related to greater degree of subcortical atrophy (beta = -0.217, P = 0.034), but not to cortical atrophy. Furthermore, TL was 371 bp shorter (P = 0.041) in participants exhibiting subcortical WMHs, and 552 bp shorter (P = 0.009) in older participants exhibiting periventricular WMHs. Conclusion: this study provides the first evidence of leukocyte TL being associated with cerebral subcortical atrophy and WMHs, lending further support to the concept of TL as a marker of biological aging, and in particular that of the aging brain.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Result 1-6 of 6

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Close

Copy and save the link in order to return to this view